Diachronic Typology and Linguistic Diversity

A brief overview of
diachronic typology
Eitan Grossman (Hebrew University of Jerusalem)
Academia Grammaticorum Salensis 24-30 July 2022
Some questions
Why are linguistic properties distributed the way they are across
languages in time and space (Greenberg 1978, Nichols 1992, Bickel
2007)?
Why are some linguistic properties common, and others rare
(Greenberg 1978, Harris 2008, Wohlgemuth & Cysouw 2009)?
It is probably easier to explain the universal absence or the universal
presence of a property, but such absolute universals are often rare, or
are trivial or even simply definitional criteria of language, and often
have more to do with communication than with linguistic structure.
T
h
e
 
p
a
s
t
 
t
e
n
s
e
 
i
n
 
a
 
s
a
m
p
l
e
 
o
f
 
t
h
e
 
w
o
r
l
d
s
 
l
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
s
(
D
a
h
l
 
&
 
V
e
l
u
p
i
l
l
a
i
 
2
0
1
3
)
S
o
u
r
c
e
s
 
o
f
 
c
r
o
s
s
l
i
n
g
u
i
s
t
i
c
 
f
r
e
q
u
e
n
c
y
 
a
s
y
m
m
e
t
r
i
e
s
?
C
o
n
s
t
r
a
i
n
t
s
 
o
n
 
p
r
o
c
e
s
s
i
n
g
?
C
o
n
s
t
r
a
i
n
t
s
 
o
n
 
m
e
m
o
r
y
?
C
o
n
s
t
r
a
i
n
t
s
 
o
n
 
l
e
a
r
n
a
b
i
l
i
t
y
?
C
o
n
s
t
r
a
i
n
t
s
 
o
n
 
a
r
t
i
c
u
l
a
t
i
o
n
?
C
o
n
s
t
r
a
i
n
t
s
 
o
n
 
p
e
r
c
e
p
t
i
o
n
?
G
e
n
e
t
i
c
a
l
l
y
-
d
e
t
e
r
m
i
n
e
d
 
U
n
i
v
e
r
s
a
l
 
G
r
a
m
m
a
r
?
W
h
a
t
 
a
b
o
u
t
 
c
o
n
s
t
r
a
i
n
t
s
 
o
n
 
s
o
c
i
a
l
 
i
n
t
e
r
a
c
t
i
o
n
?
But how do these constraints translate into
language structure?
I
s
 
t
h
e
r
e
 
a
 
d
i
r
e
c
t
 
m
a
p
p
i
n
g
 
b
e
t
w
e
e
n
 
c
o
g
n
i
t
i
o
n
 
o
r
 
p
h
y
s
i
o
l
o
g
y
 
a
n
d
 
l
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
s
t
r
u
c
t
u
r
e
s
,
 
o
r
 
i
s
 
t
h
i
s
 
c
o
n
n
e
c
t
i
o
n
 
m
e
d
i
a
t
e
d
 
s
o
m
e
h
o
w
,
 
a
n
d
 
i
f
 
s
o
,
 
b
y
 
w
h
a
t
?
An underappreciated aspect of Greenbergian
typology
W
h
a
t
 
m
e
d
i
a
t
e
s
 
b
e
t
w
e
e
n
 
c
o
g
n
i
t
i
o
n
/
p
h
y
s
i
o
l
o
g
y
 
a
n
d
 
l
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
 
s
t
r
u
c
t
u
r
e
s
i
s
 
l
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
.
Greenberg (1978)
“If a particular phenomenon can arise very frequently and is highly
stable once it occurs, it should be universal or near universal (…). If it
tends to come into existence often and in various ways, but its stability
is low, it should be found fairly often but distributed relatively evenly
among genetic linguistic stocks. […] If a particular property rarely arises
but is highly stable when it arises, it should be fairly frequent on a global
scale but be largely confined to a few genetic stocks. If it occurs only
rarely and is unstable when it occurs, it should be highly infrequent or
non-existent or sporadic in its genealogical and genetic distribution”
(Greenberg 1978: 76).
Diachronic typology
Diachronic typology is the typological study of processes of language
change and their causes.
Diachronic typology
Cross-linguistically recurrent categories are generally assumed to be the
result of cross-linguistically recurrent patterns of grammaticalization or
other general and regular processes of change.
Joan Bybee, William Pagliuca & Revere Perkins (1994), 
The Evolution of Grammar. Tense, aspect and
modality in the languages of the world
. Chicago.
Diachronic typology
Cross-linguistically recurrent sound patterns are generally assumed to
be the result of cross-linguistically recurrent patterns of sound change
and retention.
For example, nasal vowels are typically the result of VN sequences in
which the vowel is contextually nasalized, and the nasal consonant is
eventually lost. The nasality of the vowel can subsequently be lost
(Greenberg 1969).
Diachronic typology
More broadly, synchronic distributions are the result of biases in language change, which
are in turn rooted in a variety of factors, including usage biases.
Joseph Greenberg
Talmy Givón
Charles Ferguson
John Ohala
Joan Bybee
Juliette Blevins
Spike Gildea
Andrea Sansò
Sonia Cristofaro
Diachronic typology
 
More broadly, synchronic distributions are the result of biases in language change,
which are in turn rooted in a variety of factors, including usage biases.
 
For Greenberg (1969, 1978) and Bybee (2006 and elsewhere), diachronic universals
are more robust than synchronic ones – synchronic states are epiphenomenal.
 
For Blevins (2004), a principled diachronic explanation makes a synchronic
explanation redundant.
 
Similar ideas in Cristofaro (2017, 2019), who argues for a source-oriented approach
to synchronic distributions.
 
Focusing the question
Why do some categories become conventionalized (grammaticalized or
phonemicized) in many – but not all – languages?
A possible answer
From the point of view of diachronic typology, cross-linguistically common
categories are common because they involve (Grossman 2016, Grossman et al
2018, Grossman & Polis 2018)):
1.
T
Y
P
E
:
 
t
h
e
 
t
y
p
e
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
 
i
s
 
c
o
m
m
o
n
 
(
v
s
.
 
r
a
r
e
 
t
y
p
e
s
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
)
2.
P
A
T
H
:
 
m
u
l
t
i
p
l
e
 
p
a
t
h
w
a
y
s
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
 
t
h
a
t
 
c
o
n
v
e
r
g
e
 
(
v
s
.
 
a
 
s
i
n
g
l
e
 
p
a
t
h
w
a
y
)
3.
S
T
A
G
E
:
 
o
n
e
-
s
t
e
p
 
o
r
 
s
i
m
p
l
e
 
p
a
t
h
w
a
y
s
 
o
f
 
d
e
v
e
l
o
p
m
e
n
t
 
(
v
s
.
 
m
a
n
y
-
s
t
e
p
 
o
r
c
o
m
p
l
e
x
 
p
a
t
h
w
a
y
s
)
4.
S
O
U
R
C
E
:
 
c
o
m
m
o
n
 
s
o
u
r
c
e
 
c
o
n
s
t
r
u
c
t
i
o
n
s
 
v
s
.
 
r
a
r
e
 
s
o
u
r
c
e
 
c
o
n
s
t
r
u
c
t
i
o
n
s
5.
S
T
A
B
I
L
I
T
Y
:
 
o
n
c
e
 
g
r
a
m
m
a
t
i
c
a
l
i
z
e
d
,
 
t
h
e
 
c
a
t
e
g
o
r
y
 
t
y
p
e
 
t
e
n
d
s
 
t
o
 
b
e
 
s
t
a
b
l
e
6.
D
I
F
F
U
S
A
B
I
L
I
T
Y
:
 
 
t
h
e
 
c
a
t
e
g
o
r
y
 
s
p
r
e
a
d
s
 
e
a
s
i
l
y
 
v
i
a
 
l
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
 
c
o
n
t
a
c
t
.
 
Mutatis mutandis
The more that these factors converge, the more common a particular
category will be, cross-linguistically.
And cross-linguistic rarities can be explained by the convergence of
these factors, but in reverse.
Some brief examples from phonology
Northwest Mekeo (Austronesian) lacks any coronal segments, the result of 3 distinct
processes of change, some of which are rare. However, the language is now
developing allophonic coronals, so the state of having no coronals seems to be
inherently unstable (Blevins 2009).
Aspirated fricatives are very rare (Jacques 2011). Interestingly, they can develop
along multiple pathways from common sources, but they seem to be perceptually
similar to other sounds, which leads them to quickly merge.
Similarly, Nikolaev & Grossman (2018) show that affricate-dense inventories develop
from common sources through common types of change, but they are unstable
unless supported by language contact, i.e., they are lost when isolated areally.
Some brief examples from phonology
On the other hand, Bybee & Easterday (2022) show that while ejectives can develop
from common source patterns in relatively simple processes (like the fusion of a
voiceless obstruent with a following glottal stop), there is little evidence for such
changes to be the source of most ejectives in the world’s languages.
Ejectives may simply be very old and very stable in the parts of the world where they
are documented.
Summary
In contrast to traditional (and some contemporary) views, the cross-
linguistic frequency or rarity of a category is not simple a reflection of
cognitive, physical, or social biases or constraints.
Rather, cross-linguistic frequency distributions are the result of different
factors, which may act independently and interact.
Summary
1.
T
Y
P
E
:
 
t
h
e
 
t
y
p
e
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
 
i
s
 
c
o
m
m
o
n
 
(
v
s
.
 
r
a
r
e
 
t
y
p
e
s
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
)
2.
P
A
T
H
:
 
m
u
l
t
i
p
l
e
 
p
a
t
h
w
a
y
s
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
 
t
h
a
t
 
c
o
n
v
e
r
g
e
 
(
v
s
.
 
a
 
s
i
n
g
l
e
p
a
t
h
w
a
y
)
3.
S
T
A
G
E
:
 
o
n
e
-
s
t
e
p
 
o
r
 
s
i
m
p
l
e
 
p
a
t
h
w
a
y
s
 
o
f
 
d
e
v
e
l
o
p
m
e
n
t
 
(
v
s
.
 
m
a
n
y
-
s
t
e
p
o
r
 
c
o
m
p
l
e
x
 
p
a
t
h
w
a
y
s
)
4.
S
O
U
R
C
E
:
 
c
o
m
m
o
n
 
s
o
u
r
c
e
 
c
o
n
s
t
r
u
c
t
i
o
n
s
 
v
s
.
 
r
a
r
e
 
s
o
u
r
c
e
 
c
o
n
s
t
r
u
c
t
i
o
n
s
5.
S
T
A
B
I
L
I
T
Y
:
 
o
n
c
e
 
g
r
a
m
m
a
t
i
c
a
l
i
z
e
d
,
 
t
h
e
 
c
a
t
e
g
o
r
y
 
t
y
p
e
 
t
e
n
d
s
 
t
o
 
b
e
s
t
a
b
l
e
6.
D
I
F
F
U
S
A
B
I
L
I
T
Y
:
 
 
t
h
e
 
c
a
t
e
g
o
r
y
 
s
p
r
e
a
d
s
 
e
a
s
i
l
y
 
v
i
a
 
l
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
 
c
o
n
t
a
c
t
.
Universally dispreferred
structures through change
The diachrony of affix ordering in Egyptian-Coptic
Eitan Grossman (Hebrew University of Jerusalem)  & Stéphane Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
Academia Grammaticorum Salensis 24-30 July 2022
Outline of the talk
I
n
t
r
o
d
u
c
t
i
o
n
O
b
s
e
r
v
a
t
i
o
n
.
 
A
 
w
o
r
l
d
w
i
d
e
 
p
r
e
f
e
r
e
n
c
e
 
f
o
r
 
s
u
f
f
i
x
e
s
 
a
s
 
o
p
p
o
s
e
d
 
t
o
 
a
f
f
i
x
e
s
A
r
g
u
m
e
n
t
.
 
R
a
r
e
 
o
r
 
u
n
i
v
e
r
s
a
l
l
y
 
d
i
s
p
r
e
f
e
r
r
e
d
 
s
t
r
u
c
t
u
r
e
s
 
c
a
n
 
a
n
d
 
d
o
 
a
r
i
s
e
 
a
s
 
t
h
e
 
r
e
s
u
l
t
 
o
f
 
r
e
g
u
l
a
r
 
l
a
n
g
u
a
g
e
c
h
a
n
g
e
,
 
g
i
v
e
n
 
t
h
e
 
r
i
g
h
t
 
b
a
c
k
g
r
o
u
n
d
 
s
t
r
u
c
t
u
r
e
s
 
a
s
 
t
h
e
 
p
a
r
t
i
c
u
l
a
r
 
e
c
o
l
o
g
y
 
i
n
 
w
h
i
c
h
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
 
t
a
k
e
s
 
p
l
a
c
e
C
a
s
e
-
s
t
u
d
y
:
 
E
g
y
p
t
i
a
n
-
C
o
p
t
i
c
 
(
A
f
r
o
a
s
i
a
t
i
c
)
Long-term diachronic macro-change from mixed suffixing-prefixing to an 
overwhelming preference for
prefixing
C
o
n
c
l
u
s
i
o
n
s
Each of the micro-changes implicated in this macro-change are better understood in terms of changes at
the level of individual constructions, via grammaticalization, rather than in terms of a broad Sapirian
‘drift.’
These micro-changes take place at different times and have different rates
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
22
A worldwide preference for
suffixes as opposed to prefixes
 
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
23
Preference for suffixes
There is a worldwide preference for suffixes as opposed to prefixes in a proportion of about 3 to 1
in the languages of the world (Bybee et al. 1990, Cysouw 2009, Greenberg 1957, Hall 1998,
Hawkins & Cutler 1988, Himmelmann 2014, Sapir 1921, and more)
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
24
Preference for suffixes
There is a worldwide preference for suffixes as opposed to prefixes in a proportion of about 3 to 1
in the languages of the world (Bybee et al. 1990, Cysouw 2009, Greenberg 1957, Hall 1998,
Hawkins & Cutler 1988, Himmelmann 2014, Sapir 1921, and more)
Two distinct universal preferences
 (
Himmelmann 2014): grammatical morphemes have a significant
tendency
(a) to be postposed and
(b) to be bound, i.e., suffixes
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
25
Table 1. 
The suffixing preference in verbal grammatical elements from 71 languages
(Himmelmann 2014, from the database of Bybee et al. 1990: 5)
  
Preference for suffixes
 
Possible explanations
.
some form of Universal Grammar
language contact (but see Seifart 2015)
processing or some other cognitive mechanism (Cutler, Hawkins & Gillingan 1986, Caballero et al. 2008)
a correlation with the 
linear order of major constituents
 (Jacques 2013)
a world-wide retention from Proto-World
 (cf. 
Gell-Mann & Ruhlen 2011
)
processes of language change, e.g., grammaticalization (Givón 1971, Bybee 1985, Bybee et. al 1990),
perhaps due to online usage factors (Hall 1988, Himmelmann 2014)
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
26
Preference for suffixes
Explanations provided (so far)
‘it does not seem to be a fruitful approach to consider the suffixation preference as a monolithic
observation to be explained by one overarching theory of linguistic affixation’ (Cysouw 2003).
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
27
Preference for suffixes
Explanations provided (so far)
‘it does not seem to be a fruitful approach to consider the suffixation preference as a monolithic
observation to be explained by one overarching theory of linguistic affixation’ (Cysouw 2003).
Macro-characteristics of individual languages (e.g., Nichols 1986, Haspelmath et al. 2014) result
from generalizations made over individual constructions
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
28
Preference for suffixes
This is captured by Dryer’s (2013) method for comparing the degree to which languages are
characterized by a preference for prefixing, suffixing, or neither.
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
29
Table 2. 
Types of inflexional affixes (Dryer 2013)
 
Preference for suffixes
Following Dryer (2013), a language receives
a single point for prefixing or suffixing if it is 
predominantly
 prefixing or suffixing for a given parameter,
half a point for each if it has both prefixing and suffixing, with 
neither deemed dominant
no point when there is no affixing for a given parameter
The first three parameters (case affixes on nouns, subject and tense-aspect affixes in verbs) are
deemed especially important: their score is doubled
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
30
Preference for suffixes
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
31
Table 3. 
Suffixing vs Prefixing in Inflectional
Morphology (Dryer 2003)
Preference for suffixes
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
32
Table 3. 
Suffixing vs Prefixing in Inflectional
Morphology (Dryer 2003)
Interim summary
As far as inflectional morphemes are concerned, typology
corroborates a worldwide preference for postposed and suffixed
elements.
Languages that are predominantly prefixing are rare.
Ancient Egyptian-Coptic
Long-term changes towards a predominantly prefixing language
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
34
Ancient Egyptian-Coptic
Background information
An independent branch of the Afroasiatic phylum or ‘quasi-stock’
F
irst attested at the end of the fourth millenium BCE, and documented continually until sometime in the
13
th
 or 14
th
 century CE, when all of its speakers shifted to Arabic
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
35
Ancient Egyptian-Coptic
Background information
Standardly divided into five stages and and two macro-phases
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
36
Table 4. 
Stages of Egyptian-Coptic (as discussed here
)
Ancient Egyptian-Coptic
Background information
Standardly divided into five stages and and two macro-phases
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
37
Table 4. 
Stages of Egyptian-Coptic (as discussed here
)
Ancient Egyptian-Coptic
Background information
Coptic is a predominantly prefixing language
Earlier Egyptian is not:
Dominant VSO order of major constituents
A small number (1-3) of postposed or suffixed inflectional
morphemes on nouns and verbs
A diachronic trend from synthetic to analytic to agglutination and
polysynthesis (
anasynthesis
, Haspelmath 2011)
A diachronic trend towards prefixed grammatical elements
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
38
Ancient Egyptian-Coptic
39
Table 5. 
Calculation of the affixing index for Coptic
Ancient Egyptian-Coptic
40
Table 5. 
Calculation of the affixing index for Coptic
Ancient Egyptian-Coptic
41
Table 5. 
Calculation of the affixing index for Coptic
Ancient Egyptian-Coptic
Background information
Coptic is a predominantly prefixing language
With its extremely high prefixing preference (12/13), Coptic belongs to the
rare 6% or so of languages that are predominantly prefixing.
Moreover, it has a higher prefixing index than any other language in Dryer’s
969-language sample. (The closest competitor is Hunde [Bantu; Democratic
Republic of Congo; Kahombo 1992], with a prefixing index of 9.5/13.)
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
42
Ancient Egyptian-Coptic
Background information
Coptic is a predominantly prefixing language
With its extremely high prefixing preference (12/13), Coptic belongs to the rare
6% or so of languages that are predominantly prefixing.
Moreover, it has a higher prefixing index than any other language in Dryer’s
969-language sample. (The closest competitor is Hunde [Bantu; Democratic
Republic of Congo; Kahombo 1992], with a prefixing index of 9.5/13.)
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
43
Coptic is an areal outlier
While predominantly
prefixing languages are
relatively common in
Mesoamerica and in Africa,
within Africa it is only in
western and southern sub-
Saharan Africa that
predominant prefixing is
common
A
 
t
e
m
p
l
a
t
e
 
f
o
r
 
n
o
u
n
 
m
o
r
p
h
o
s
y
n
t
a
x
A
 
t
e
m
p
l
a
t
e
 
f
o
r
 
v
e
r
b
 
m
o
r
p
h
o
s
y
n
t
a
x
 
The diachrony of affix ordering
Methodology
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
46
For diachronic purposes, a modified form of Dryer’s typology is useful.
Rather than limiting the score to 0 for no affix, 1 for either suffixing or prefixing,
and 0.5 for both prefixing and affixing, we suggest using two scales, both of
which admit a more fine-grained analysis
The diachrony of affix ordering
Methodology
47
Table 6. 
A finer-grained index for affixing
Coptic
Prefixed case markers (on postverbal
noun phrases in S, A, or P role)
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
48
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 1 
 Case affixes on nouns
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Prefixed case markers (on postverbal
noun phrases in S, A, or P role)
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
49
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 1 
 Case affixes on nouns
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Prefixed case markers (on postverbal
noun phrases in S, A, or P role)
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
50
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 1 
 Case affixes on nouns
T
y
p
e
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
:
 
(
s
e
c
o
n
d
a
r
y
)
 
g
r
a
m
m
a
t
i
c
a
l
i
z
a
t
i
o
n
 
(
f
r
o
m
 
p
r
e
p
o
s
i
t
i
o
n
 
t
o
 
a
c
c
u
s
a
t
i
v
e
 
c
a
s
e
m
a
r
k
e
r
,
 
a
n
d
 
f
r
o
m
 
a
n
t
i
t
o
p
i
c
 
m
a
r
k
e
r
 
t
o
 
n
o
m
i
n
a
t
i
v
e
 
m
a
r
k
e
r
)
C
o
p
t
i
c
Prefixed subject affixes on verbs
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
51
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 2 
 Subject affixes on verbs
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Prefixed subject affixes on verbs
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
52
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 2 
 Subject affixes on verbs
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Prefixed subject affixes on verbs
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
53
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 2 
 Subject affixes on verbs
T
y
p
e
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
:
 
d
e
v
e
l
o
p
m
e
n
t
 
o
f
 
a
 
n
e
w
 
p
r
o
n
o
u
n
 
p
a
r
a
d
i
g
m
 
(
w
h
i
c
h
 
g
r
a
d
u
a
l
l
y
 
b
e
c
o
m
e
s
 
a
s
u
b
j
e
c
t
 
p
r
e
f
i
x
)
 
a
n
d
 
t
h
e
 
s
h
i
f
t
 
o
f
 
a
 
m
i
n
o
r
 
u
s
a
g
e
 
p
a
t
t
e
r
n
 
(
a
 
p
e
r
i
p
h
r
a
s
t
i
c
 
c
o
n
s
t
r
u
c
t
i
o
n
i
n
v
o
l
v
i
n
g
 
a
n
 
a
u
x
i
l
i
a
r
y
)
 
t
o
 
a
 
m
a
j
o
r
 
p
a
t
t
e
r
n
Coptic
Prefixed tense-aspect affixes on verbs
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
54
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 3 
 Tense-aspect affixes on verbs
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Prefixed tense-aspect affixes on verbs
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
55
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 3 
 Tense-aspect affixes on verbs
Anasynthesis of the verb
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Prefixed tense-aspect affixes on verbs
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
57
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 3 
 Tense-aspect affixes on verbs
T
y
p
e
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
:
 
O
l
d
 
t
e
n
s
e
-
a
s
p
e
c
t
 
s
u
f
f
i
x
e
s
 
a
r
e
 
l
o
s
t
,
 
w
h
i
l
e
 
n
e
w
 
T
A
M
 
m
a
r
k
e
r
s
 
a
r
e
g
r
a
m
m
a
t
i
c
a
l
i
z
e
d
 
f
r
o
m
 
a
u
x
i
l
i
a
r
y
 
v
e
r
b
s
 
i
n
 
p
e
r
i
p
h
r
a
s
t
i
c
 
c
o
n
s
t
r
u
c
t
i
o
n
s
.
Since the linear order of these grammaticalizing constructions follows that of basic
verbal clauses, i.e., VSO/AuxSV, the result is tense-aspect prefixes.
C
o
p
t
i
c
Productive: prefixed plural markers
Non-productive: 
plural-suffixing
construction (which often involves
stem-internal alternations)
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
58
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 4 
 Plural affixes on nouns
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Productive: prefixed plural markers
Non-productive: 
plural-suffixing
construction (which often involves
stem-internal alternations)
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
59
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 4 
 Plural affixes on nouns
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
Coptic
Productive: prefixed plural markers
Non-productive: 
plural-suffixing
construction (which often involves
stem-internal alternations)
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
60
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 4 
 Plural affixes on nouns
T
y
p
e
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
:
 
e
m
e
r
g
e
n
c
e
 
o
f
 
n
e
w
p
r
e
p
o
s
e
d
 
a
r
t
i
c
l
e
s
 
t
h
a
t
 
u
n
a
m
b
i
g
u
o
u
s
l
y
m
a
r
k
 
n
u
m
b
e
r
,
 
a
n
d
 
l
o
s
s
 
o
f
 
p
l
u
r
a
l
 
s
u
f
f
i
x
e
s
.
C
o
p
t
i
c
Productive: prefixed possessives on
nouns
Non-productive: suffixed possessives 
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
61
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 5 
 Possessive affixes on nouns
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Productive: prefixed possessives on
nouns
Non-productive: suffixed possessives 
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
62
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 5 
 Possessive affixes on nouns
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
Coptic
Productive: prefixed possessives on
nouns
Non-productive: suffixed possessives 
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
63
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 5 
 Possessive affixes on nouns
T
y
p
e
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
:
 
e
m
e
r
g
e
n
c
e
 
o
f
 
n
e
w
p
r
e
p
o
s
e
d
 
a
r
t
i
c
l
e
s
 
t
h
a
t
 
u
n
a
m
b
i
g
u
o
u
s
l
y
m
a
r
k
 
n
u
m
b
e
r
,
 
a
n
d
 
l
o
s
s
 
o
f
 
p
l
u
r
a
l
 
s
u
f
f
i
x
e
s
.
Coptic
Definite and indefinite prefixes on
nouns
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
64
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 6 
 (In)definite affixes on nouns
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Definite and indefinite prefixes on
nouns
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
65
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 6 
 (In)definite affixes on nouns
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Definite and indefinite prefixes on
nouns
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
66
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 6 
 (In)definite affixes on nouns
T
y
p
e
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
:
 
G
r
a
m
m
a
t
i
c
a
l
i
z
a
t
i
o
n
(
d
e
m
o
n
s
t
r
a
t
i
v
e
 
>
 
d
e
f
i
n
i
t
e
 
a
r
t
i
c
l
e
 
>
 
d
e
f
i
n
i
t
e
a
f
f
i
x
;
 
o
n
e
 
>
 
i
n
d
e
f
i
n
i
t
e
 
a
r
t
i
c
l
e
 
>
 
i
n
d
e
f
i
n
i
t
e
a
f
f
i
x
)
 
;
 
I
n
d
e
p
e
n
d
e
n
t
 
w
o
r
d
 
>
 
a
f
f
i
x
Coptic
Suffixed pronominal object on verbs
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
67
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 7 
 Pron. object affixes on verbs
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Suffixed pronominal object on verbs
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
68
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 7 
 Pron. object affixes on verbs
Table 7. Two series of bound person markers
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Suffixed pronominal object on verbs
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
69
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 7 
 Pron. object affixes on verbs
T
y
p
e
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
:
 
s
h
i
f
t
 
f
r
o
m
 
a
 
m
i
n
o
r
 
u
s
a
g
e
 
p
a
t
t
e
r
n
 
t
o
 
a
 
m
a
j
o
r
 
o
n
e
.
 
T
h
e
 
s
u
f
f
i
x
 
t
a
k
e
o
v
e
r
 
i
s
 
t
h
e
r
e
s
u
l
t
 
o
f
 
g
r
a
m
m
a
t
i
c
a
l
i
z
a
t
i
o
n
 
o
f
 
v
e
r
b
 
f
o
r
m
s
 
w
i
t
h
 
s
u
f
f
i
x
e
d
 
P
 
m
a
r
k
e
r
s
,
 
a
n
d
 
t
h
e
 
l
o
s
s
 
o
f
 
v
e
r
b
f
o
r
m
s
 
w
i
t
h
 
c
l
i
t
i
c
 
P
 
m
a
r
k
e
r
s
,
 
t
h
e
r
e
 
i
s
 
n
o
 
c
l
i
t
i
c
-
t
o
-
a
f
f
i
x
 
g
r
a
m
m
a
t
i
c
a
l
i
z
a
t
i
o
n
 
i
n
v
o
l
v
e
d
 
(
H
o
p
p
e
r
&
 
T
r
a
u
g
o
t
t
 
2
0
0
3
,
 
H
i
m
m
e
l
m
a
n
n
 
2
0
1
4
)
.
C
o
p
t
i
c
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
70
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 8 
 Negative affixes on verbs
Portmanteau prefixes that
code both TAM values and
polarity
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
71
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 8 
 Negative affixes on verbs
Portmanteau prefixes that
code both TAM values and
polarity
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
72
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 8 
 Negative affixes on verbs
Portmanteau prefixes
that code both TAM
values and polarity
T
y
p
e
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
:
 
f
r
o
m
 
L
a
t
e
 
E
g
y
p
t
i
a
n
 
o
n
w
a
r
d
s
,
 
n
e
g
a
t
i
o
n
s
 
i
n
 
m
a
i
n
 
v
e
r
b
a
l
 
c
l
a
u
s
e
s
 
b
e
g
a
n
 
t
o
 
b
e
u
n
i
v
e
r
b
a
t
e
d
 
w
i
t
h
 
T
A
M
 
a
u
x
i
l
i
a
r
i
e
s
 
(
u
n
i
v
e
r
b
a
t
i
o
n
,
 
g
r
a
m
m
a
t
i
c
a
l
i
z
a
t
i
o
n
 
o
f
 
p
o
r
t
m
a
n
t
e
a
u
T
A
M
/
P
o
l
a
r
i
t
y
 
p
r
e
f
i
x
e
s
)
The development of negative prefixes
The Egyptian-Coptic nondumitive (‘not yet’)
C
o
p
t
i
c
Unmarked direct yes/no questions
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
74
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 9 
 Interrogative affixes on verbs
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Unmarked direct yes/no questions
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
75
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 9 
 Interrogative affixes on verbs
No prefixed or suffixed
interrogation markers
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 9 
 Interrogative affixes on verbs
From focus construction to interrogative marker
Stage 1: Wh-questions are typically constructed as cleft sentences
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 9 
 Interrogative affixes on verbs
From focus construction to interrogative marker
Stage 2: Same, but with periphrasis based on the verb 
iri
 ‘to do’
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 9 
 Interrogative affixes on verbs
From focus construction to interrogative marker
Stage 3: Earlier nominalization opaque, just a focus-marking prefix
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 9 
 Interrogative affixes on verbs
From focus construction to interrogative marker
Stage 4: Focus-marking prefix extended to yes-no questions
T
y
p
e
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
:
 
s
e
c
o
n
d
a
r
y
 
g
r
a
m
m
a
t
i
c
a
l
i
z
a
t
i
o
n
 
(
f
r
o
m
n
o
m
i
n
a
l
i
z
e
r
 
t
o
 
f
o
c
u
s
 
t
o
 
i
n
t
e
r
r
o
g
a
t
i
v
e
 
p
r
e
f
i
x
C
o
p
t
i
c
Coptic has a set of verbal prefixes that
indicate subordinate-clause status
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
80
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 10 
 Adv. subordinator affixes on 
verbs
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Coptic has a set of verbal prefixes that
indicate subordinate-clause status
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
81
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 10 
 Adv. subordinator affixes on 
verbs
 
Interestingly, Dryer (2013) claims that
adverbial subordination prefixes don’t exist.
Grossman, Antonov & Jacques (2018) point
out that they do exist.
Dryer (2021) also argues that they exist, but
doesn;’t mention earlier work.
P
r
e
v
i
o
u
s
 
s
t
a
g
e
s
C
o
p
t
i
c
Coptic has a set of verbal prefixes that
indicate subordinate-clause status
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
82
The diachrony of affix ordering
Parameter 10 
 Adv. subordinator affixes on 
verbs
T
y
p
e
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
:
 
f
r
o
m
 
L
a
t
e
 
E
g
y
p
t
i
a
n
 
o
n
w
a
r
d
s
,
 
c
l
a
u
s
e
-
i
n
i
t
i
a
l
 
c
o
n
j
u
n
c
t
i
o
n
s
 
b
e
g
i
n
 
t
o
 
b
e
u
n
i
v
e
r
b
a
t
e
d
 
w
i
t
h
 
a
u
x
i
l
i
a
r
y
 
v
e
r
b
s
,
 
c
r
e
a
t
i
n
g
,
 
i
n
 
e
f
f
e
c
t
,
 
a
d
v
e
r
b
i
a
l
 
s
u
b
o
r
d
i
n
a
t
o
r
 
p
r
e
f
i
x
e
s
o
n
 
v
e
r
b
s
 
(
g
r
a
m
m
a
t
i
c
a
l
i
z
a
t
i
o
n
,
 
v
i
a
 
p
e
r
i
p
h
r
a
s
i
s
 
a
n
d
 
u
n
i
v
e
r
b
a
t
i
o
n
)
Universally disprefered structures
through change
Conclusions
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
83
Types of change and stages
84
Table 8. Summary of types of
changes, period of emergence
and of full grammaticalization
Types of change and stages
85
Table 8. Summary of types of changes, period of
emergence and of full grammaticalization
Grammaticalization
Types of change and stages
86
Table 8. Summary of types of changes, period of
emergence and of full grammaticalization
Grammaticalization
Minor to major patterns
Types of change and stages
87
Table 8. Summary of types of changes, period of
emergence and of full grammaticalization
Grammaticalization
Replacement
Minor to major patterns
Types of change and stages
88
Table 8. Summary of types of changes, period of
emergence and of full grammaticalization
Grammaticalization
Replacement
Minor to major patterns
Loss of suffixes
The diachrony of affixing preferences
89
The diachrony of affixing preferences
90
Contact?
91
G
r
o
s
s
m
a
n
 
(
2
0
1
8
)
Conclusions
Directly relevant for larger questions, such as the relationship between language universals and
language change (e.g., Good 2008): do synchronic structural universals constrain change, or do
diachronic universals, ultimately motivated by synchronic usage
 
factors, give rise to synchronic
universals?
Apparently ‘counter-directional’ changes are crucial here: why should language change lead to
universally dispreferred distributions of linguistic structures?
In brief, language change seems to be indifferent to typological distributions.
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
92
Conclusions
 
Ancient Egyptian-Coptic (Afroasiatic) shows a long-term diachronic macro-change from mixed
suffixing-prefixing to an overwhelming preference for prefixing.
Changes in affix order in Ancient Egyptian-Coptic occur at different times, at different rates, and to
different degrees in different domains.
Crucially, there is nothing unusual about the actual processes of change themselves; what 
may
 be
unusual, from a cross-linguistic point of view, is the length of uninterrupted documentation of a
single language, which allows us to observe long-term changes with abundant evidence.
Other factors include (rare) VSO order in Earlier Egyptian,
As such, while Coptic-Egyptian seems to be swimming against the typological tide, it is just paddling
along, construction by construction, with regular processes of language change.
E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS)
93
Once again
1.
T
Y
P
E
:
 
t
h
e
 
t
y
p
e
s
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
 
a
r
e
 
n
o
t
 
t
h
e
m
s
e
l
v
e
s
 
u
n
u
s
u
a
l
 
o
r
 
r
a
r
e
 
f
o
r
 
t
h
e
 
m
o
s
t
p
a
r
t
.
2.
P
A
T
H
:
 
m
u
l
t
i
p
l
e
 
p
a
t
h
w
a
y
s
 
o
f
 
c
h
a
n
g
e
 
t
h
a
t
 
c
o
n
v
e
r
g
e
 
(
v
s
.
 
a
 
s
i
n
g
l
e
 
p
a
t
h
w
a
y
)
3.
S
T
A
G
E
:
 
 
m
o
s
t
l
y
 
m
a
n
y
-
s
t
e
p
 
o
r
 
c
o
m
p
l
e
x
 
p
a
t
h
w
a
y
s
4.
S
O
U
R
C
E
:
 
m
o
s
t
 
o
f
 
t
h
e
 
s
o
u
r
c
e
 
c
o
n
s
t
r
u
c
t
i
o
n
s
 
a
r
e
 
n
o
t
 
r
a
r
e
 
t
h
e
m
s
e
l
v
e
s
,
 
b
u
t
V
S
O
 
o
r
d
e
r
,
 
w
h
i
c
h
 
p
r
o
b
a
b
l
y
 
p
l
a
y
e
d
 
a
 
l
a
r
g
e
 
r
o
l
e
,
 
i
s
 
r
e
l
a
t
i
v
e
l
y
 
r
a
r
e
.
5.
S
T
A
B
I
L
I
T
Y
:
 
i
n
f
l
e
c
t
i
o
n
a
l
 
p
r
e
f
i
x
e
s
 
i
n
 
E
g
y
p
t
i
a
n
-
C
o
p
t
i
c
,
 
o
n
c
e
 
g
r
a
m
m
a
t
i
c
a
l
i
z
e
d
,
a
r
e
 
s
t
a
b
l
e
.
6.
D
I
F
F
U
S
A
B
I
L
I
T
Y
:
 
 
n
o
t
 
d
u
e
 
t
o
 
c
o
n
t
a
c
t
,
 
p
r
o
b
a
b
l
y
 
n
o
t
 
v
e
r
y
 
d
i
f
f
u
s
a
b
l
e
 
a
n
y
w
a
y
.
B
u
t
 
i
s
 
t
h
a
t
 
a
l
l
?
Dryer’s typological studies relied on orthographic evidence for word
division, and hence the number and nature of affixes.
But how reliable is this?
From Arkadiev (2005, ms)
From Arkadiev (2005, ms)
 
Thank you!
 
Slide Note
Embed
Share

Delve into the fascinating realm of diachronic typology and linguistic properties across languages in time and space. Uncover the distribution of linguistic features, the past tense in various languages, and the underlying constraints shaping language structures. Discover the intricate interactions between cognition, physiology, language change, and Greenbergian typology.

  • Diachronic Typology
  • Linguistic Diversity
  • Language Structure
  • Language Change
  • Cognition

Uploaded on Oct 10, 2024 | 0 Views


Download Presentation

Please find below an Image/Link to download the presentation.

The content on the website is provided AS IS for your information and personal use only. It may not be sold, licensed, or shared on other websites without obtaining consent from the author.If you encounter any issues during the download, it is possible that the publisher has removed the file from their server.

You are allowed to download the files provided on this website for personal or commercial use, subject to the condition that they are used lawfully. All files are the property of their respective owners.

The content on the website is provided AS IS for your information and personal use only. It may not be sold, licensed, or shared on other websites without obtaining consent from the author.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. A brief overview of diachronic typology Eitan Grossman (Hebrew University of Jerusalem) Academia Grammaticorum Salensis 24-30 July 2022

  2. Some questions Why are linguistic properties distributed the way they are across languages in time and space (Greenberg 1978, Nichols 1992, Bickel 2007)? Why are some linguistic properties common, and others rare (Greenberg 1978, Harris 2008, Wohlgemuth & Cysouw 2009)? It is probably easier to explain the universal absence or the universal presence of a property, but such absolute universals are often rare, or are trivial or even simply definitional criteria of language, and often have more to do with communication than with linguistic structure.

  3. The past tense in a sample of the world The past tense in a sample of the world s languages ( (Dahl & Dahl & Velupillai Velupillai 2013 s languages 2013) )

  4. Sources of Sources of crosslinguistic crosslinguistic frequency asymmetries? frequency asymmetries? Constraints on processing Constraints on memory Constraints on learnability Constraints on articulation Constraints on perception Genetically-determined Universal Grammar What about constraints on social interaction processing? memory? learnability? articulation? perception? Universal Grammar? social interaction?

  5. But how do these constraints translate into language structure? Is there a direct structures, or is this connection mediated somehow, and if so, by what? direct mapping between cognition or physiology and language

  6. An underappreciated aspect of Greenbergian typology What mediates between cognition/physiology and language structures is language change language change.

  7. Greenberg (1978) If a particular phenomenon can arise very frequently and is highly stable once it occurs, it should be universal or near universal ( ). If it tends to come into existence often and in various ways, but its stability is low, it should be found fairly often but distributed relatively evenly among genetic linguistic stocks. [ ] If a particular property rarely arises but is highly stable when it arises, it should be fairly frequent on a global scale but be largely confined to a few genetic stocks. If it occurs only rarely and is unstable when it occurs, it should be highly infrequent or non-existent or sporadic in its genealogical and genetic distribution (Greenberg 1978: 76).

  8. Diachronic typology Diachronic typology is the typological study of processes of language change and their causes.

  9. Diachronic typology Cross-linguistically recurrent categories are generally assumed to be the result of cross-linguistically recurrent patterns of grammaticalization or other general and regular processes of change. Joan Bybee, William Pagliuca & Revere Perkins (1994), The Evolution of Grammar. Tense, aspect and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago.

  10. Diachronic typology Cross-linguistically recurrent sound patterns are generally assumed to be the result of cross-linguistically recurrent patterns of sound change and retention. For example, nasal vowels are typically the result of VN sequences in which the vowel is contextually nasalized, and the nasal consonant is eventually lost. The nasality of the vowel can subsequently be lost (Greenberg 1969).

  11. Diachronic typology More broadly, synchronic distributions are the result of biases in language change, which are in turn rooted in a variety of factors, including usage biases. Joseph Greenberg Talmy Giv n Charles Ferguson John Ohala Joan Bybee Juliette Blevins Spike Gildea Andrea Sans Sonia Cristofaro

  12. Diachronic typology More broadly, synchronic distributions are the result of biases in language change, which are in turn rooted in a variety of factors, including usage biases. For Greenberg (1969, 1978) and Bybee (2006 and elsewhere), diachronic universals are more robust than synchronic ones synchronic states are epiphenomenal. For Blevins (2004), a principled diachronic explanation makes a synchronic explanation redundant. Similar ideas in Cristofaro (2017, 2019), who argues for a source-oriented approach to synchronic distributions.

  13. Focusing the question Why do some categories become conventionalized (grammaticalized or phonemicized) in many but not all languages?

  14. A possible answer From the point of view of diachronic typology, cross-linguistically common categories are common because they involve (Grossman 2016, Grossman et al 2018, Grossman & Polis 2018)): 1. 1. TYPE TYPE: the type of change is common (vs. rare types of change) 2. 2. PATH PATH: multiple pathways of change that converge (vs. a single pathway) 3. 3. STAGE STAGE: one-step or simple pathways of development (vs. many-step or complex pathways) 4. 4. SOURCE SOURCE: common source constructions vs. rare source constructions 5. 5. STABILITY STABILITY: once grammaticalized, the category type tends to be stable 6. 6. DIFFUSABILITY DIFFUSABILITY: the category spreads easily via language contact.

  15. Mutatis mutandis The more that these factors converge, the more common a particular category will be, cross-linguistically. And cross-linguistic rarities can be explained by the convergence of these factors, but in reverse.

  16. Some brief examples from phonology Northwest Mekeo (Austronesian) lacks any coronal segments, the result of 3 distinct processes of change, some of which are rare. However, the language is now developing allophonic coronals, so the state of having no coronals seems to be inherently unstable (Blevins 2009). Aspirated fricatives are very rare (Jacques 2011). Interestingly, they can develop along multiple pathways from common sources, but they seem to be perceptually similar to other sounds, which leads them to quickly merge. Similarly, Nikolaev & Grossman (2018) show that affricate-dense inventories develop from common sources through common types of change, but they are unstable unless supported by language contact, i.e., they are lost when isolated areally.

  17. Some brief examples from phonology On the other hand, Bybee & Easterday (2022) show that while ejectives can develop from common source patterns in relatively simple processes (like the fusion of a voiceless obstruent with a following glottal stop), there is little evidence for such changes to be the source of most ejectives in the world s languages. Ejectives may simply be very old and very stable in the parts of the world where they are documented.

  18. Summary In contrast to traditional (and some contemporary) views, the cross- linguistic frequency or rarity of a category is not simple a reflection of cognitive, physical, or social biases or constraints. Rather, cross-linguistic frequency distributions are the result of different factors, which may act independently and interact.

  19. Summary 1. 1. TYPE TYPE: the type of change is common (vs. rare types of change) 2. 2. PATH PATH: multiple pathways of change that converge (vs. a single pathway) 3. 3. STAGE STAGE: one-step or simple pathways of development (vs. many-step or complex pathways) 4. 4. SOURCE SOURCE: common source constructions vs. rare source constructions 5. 5. STABILITY STABILITY: once grammaticalized, the category type tends to be stable 6. 6. DIFFUSABILITY DIFFUSABILITY: the category spreads easily via language contact.

  20. Universally dispreferred structures through change The diachrony of affix ordering in Egyptian-Coptic Eitan Grossman (Hebrew University of Jerusalem) & St phane Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) Academia Grammaticorum Salensis 24-30 July 2022

  21. Outline of the talk Introduction Introduction Observation Observation. . A worldwide preference for suffixes as opposed to affixes Argument Argument. Rare or universally dispreferred structures can and do arise as the result of regular language change, given the right background structures as the particular ecology in which change takes place Case Case- -study: study: Egyptian-Coptic (Afroasiatic) Long-term diachronic macro-change from mixed suffixing-prefixing to an overwhelming preference for prefixing Conclusions Conclusions Each of the micro-changes implicated in this macro-change are better understood in terms of changes at the level of individual constructions, via grammaticalization, rather than in terms of a broad Sapirian drift. These micro-changes take place at different times and have different rates E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 22

  22. A worldwide preference for suffixes as opposed to prefixes E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 23

  23. Preference for suffixes There is a worldwide preference for suffixes as opposed to prefixes in a proportion of about 3 to 1 in the languages of the world (Bybee et al. 1990, Cysouw 2009, Greenberg 1957, Hall 1998, Hawkins & Cutler 1988, Himmelmann 2014, Sapir 1921, and more) E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 24

  24. Preference for suffixes There is a worldwide preference for suffixes as opposed to prefixes in a proportion of about 3 to 1 in the languages of the world (Bybee et al. 1990, Cysouw 2009, Greenberg 1957, Hall 1998, Hawkins & Cutler 1988, Himmelmann 2014, Sapir 1921, and more) Two distinct universal preferences (Himmelmann 2014): grammatical morphemes have a significant tendency (a) to be postposed and (b) to be bound, i.e., suffixes Table 1. The suffixing preference in verbal grammatical elements from 71 languages (Himmelmann 2014, from the database of Bybee et al. 1990: 5) E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 25

  25. Preference for suffixes Possible explanations . some form of Universal Grammar language contact (but see Seifart 2015) processing or some other cognitive mechanism (Cutler, Hawkins & Gillingan 1986, Caballero et al. 2008) a correlation with the linear order of major constituents (Jacques 2013) a world-wide retention from Proto-World (cf. Gell-Mann & Ruhlen 2011) processes of language change, e.g., grammaticalization (Giv n 1971, Bybee 1985, Bybee et. al 1990), perhaps due to online usage factors (Hall 1988, Himmelmann 2014) E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 26

  26. Preference for suffixes Explanations provided (so far) it does not seem to be a fruitful approach to consider the suffixation preference as a monolithic observation to be explained by one overarching theory of linguistic affixation (Cysouw 2003). E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 27

  27. Preference for suffixes Explanations provided (so far) it does not seem to be a fruitful approach to consider the suffixation preference as a monolithic observation to be explained by one overarching theory of linguistic affixation (Cysouw 2003). Macro-characteristics of individual languages (e.g., Nichols 1986, Haspelmath et al. 2014) result from generalizations made over individual constructions E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 28

  28. Preference for suffixes This is captured by Dryer s (2013) method for comparing the degree to which languages are characterized by a preference for prefixing, suffixing, or neither. Table 2. Types of inflexional affixes (Dryer 2013) E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 29

  29. Preference for suffixes Following Dryer (2013), a language receives a single point for prefixing or suffixing if it is predominantly prefixing or suffixing for a given parameter, half a point for each if it has both prefixing and suffixing, with neither deemed dominant no point when there is no affixing for a given parameter The first three parameters (case affixes on nouns, subject and tense-aspect affixes in verbs) are deemed especially important: their score is doubled E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 30

  30. Preference for suffixes Table 3. Suffixing vs Prefixing in Inflectional Morphology (Dryer 2003) E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 31

  31. Preference for suffixes Table 3. Suffixing vs Prefixing in Inflectional Morphology (Dryer 2003) E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 32

  32. Interim summary As far as inflectional morphemes are concerned, typology corroborates a worldwide preference for postposed and suffixed elements. Languages that are predominantly prefixing are rare.

  33. Ancient Egyptian-Coptic Long-term changes towards a predominantly prefixing language E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 34

  34. Ancient Egyptian-Coptic Background information An independent branch of the Afroasiatic phylum or quasi-stock First attested at the end of the fourth millenium BCE, and documented continually until sometime in the 13th or 14th century CE, when all of its speakers shifted to Arabic E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 35

  35. Ancient Egyptian-Coptic Background information Standardly divided into five stages and and two macro-phases Table 4. Stages of Egyptian-Coptic (as discussed here) E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 36

  36. Ancient Egyptian-Coptic Background information Standardly divided into five stages and and two macro-phases Table 4. Stages of Egyptian-Coptic (as discussed here) E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 37

  37. Ancient Egyptian-Coptic Background information Coptic is a predominantly prefixing language Earlier Egyptian is not: Dominant VSO order of major constituents A small number (1-3) of postposed or suffixed inflectional morphemes on nouns and verbs A diachronic trend from synthetic to analytic to agglutination and polysynthesis (anasynthesis, Haspelmath 2011) A diachronic trend towards prefixed grammatical elements E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 38

  38. Ancient Egyptian-Coptic Table 5. Calculation of the affixing index for Coptic 39

  39. Ancient Egyptian-Coptic Table 5. Calculation of the affixing index for Coptic 40

  40. Ancient Egyptian-Coptic Table 5. Calculation of the affixing index for Coptic 41

  41. Ancient Egyptian-Coptic Background information Coptic is a predominantly prefixing language With its extremely high prefixing preference (12/13), Coptic belongs to the rare 6% or so of languages that are predominantly prefixing. Moreover, it has a higher prefixing index than any other language in Dryer s 969-language sample. (The closest competitor is Hunde [Bantu; Democratic Republic of Congo; Kahombo 1992], with a prefixing index of 9.5/13.) E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 42

  42. Ancient Egyptian-Coptic Background information Coptic is a predominantly prefixing language With its extremely high prefixing preference (12/13), Coptic belongs to the rare 6% or so of languages that are predominantly prefixing. Moreover, it has a higher prefixing index than any other language in Dryer s 969-language sample. (The closest competitor is Hunde [Bantu; Democratic Republic of Congo; Kahombo 1992], with a prefixing index of 9.5/13.) Coptic is an areal outlier While predominantly prefixing languages are relatively common in Mesoamerica and in Africa, within Africa it is only in western and southern sub- Saharan Africa that predominant prefixing is common E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 43

  43. A template for noun morphosyntax

  44. A template for verb morphosyntax

  45. The diachrony of affix ordering Methodology For diachronic purposes, a modified form of Dryer s typology is useful. Rather than limiting the score to 0 for no affix, 1 for either suffixing or prefixing, and 0.5 for both prefixing and affixing, we suggest using two scales, both of which admit a more fine-grained analysis E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 46

  46. The diachrony of affix ordering Methodology Table 6. A finer-grained index for affixing 47

  47. The diachrony of affix ordering Parameter 1 Case affixes on nouns Coptic Prefixed case markers (on postverbal noun phrases in S, A, or P role) E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 48

  48. The diachrony of affix ordering Parameter 1 Case affixes on nouns Previous stages Previous stages Coptic Coptic Prefixed case markers (on postverbal noun phrases in S, A, or P role) E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 49

  49. The diachrony of affix ordering Parameter 1 Case affixes on nouns Previous stages Previous stages Coptic Coptic Prefixed case markers (on postverbal noun phrases in S, A, or P role) Type of change: Type of change: ( (secondary) marker, and from antitopic marker to nominative marker) secondary) grammaticalization grammaticalization (from preposition to accusative case E. Grossman (HUJi) & St. Polis (F.R.S.-FNRS) 50

Related


More Related Content

giItT1WQy@!-/#giItT1WQy@!-/#giItT1WQy@!-/#giItT1WQy@!-/#giItT1WQy@!-/#giItT1WQy@!-/#