The Fascinating Journey of Language Acquisition in Children

 
How children learn language
 
Psycholinguistics – Meeting 8
 
 
At birth we cannot comprehend speech, nor can
we produce speech. Yet, by the age of 4 years we
have learned vocabulary and grammatical rules
for creating a variety of sentence structures
including negatives, questions, and relative
clauses.
Although 4-year-olds still have passives and some
other elaborate syntactic structures to learn,
along with a never-ending stock of vocabulary
items, they have already overcome the most
difficult obstacles in language learning.
 
Vocalization to babbling
 
Prior to uttering speech sounds, infants make
a variety of sounds – crying, cooing, gurgling.
Infants everywhere seem to make the same
variety of sounds, even children who are born
deaf.
Around the seventh month, children ordinarily
begin to babble, to produce what may be
described as repeated syllables (‘syllabic
reduplication’), e.g. ‘baba’, ‘momo’, ‘panpan’.
 
 
While most of the syllables are of the basic
Consonant 
+ 
Vowel 
type (‘baba’ and ‘momo’),
some consist of closed syllables of the simple
Consonant 
+ 
Vowel 
+ 
Consonant 
variety
(‘panpan’).
 
Babbling to speech
 
It is from the advanced stage of babbling that
children move into uttering their first words.
Often this occurs at around 1 year of age but
can occur much earlier or much later.
 
How infants perceive speech
 
About 3 months before birth, you begin
sensing sound and draw on this experience
from the outset. You begin life with a
preference for listening to speech over other
complex sounds.
How do infants break into the speech stream?
They face three challenges:
 
 
The segmentation problem
: How do infants figure out that
a stream of speech can be segmented into phonemes?
The invariance problem
: How do infants identify a stable set
of phonemes from a signal that is full of variation? For
example, /p/ is pronounced differently according to
whether it appears at the beginning or end of a word:
compare 
pit 
(where /p/ is aspirated) to 
tip 
(where /p/ isn't
aspirated). Add to that the fact that each speaker has a
slightly different pronunciation, and that even the same
speaker doesn't pronounce the same word twice in the
same way.
The language problem
: How do infants figure out which set
of sounds belong to their target language?
 
 
Initially young infants can discriminate (perceive
whether two sounds are the same or different)
phonetic sounds from any human language, even ones
they haven't been exposed to.
They retain this ability until the age of about 8 months,
at which point they show a preference for the sounds
of their target language.
Infants pay more attention to the sounds that are most
frequent. Being able to discriminate the sounds of a
target language and to identify the most frequent ones
are the first steps in solving the segmentation and to
language problems.
 
 
By 8 months, infants are expert statisticians and
keep score of which sounds hang out together.
They detect regularities about which sounds are
likely to occur with which other sounds, the
typical stress patterns on words, and the
difference between content words (such as verbs
and nouns) and function words (articles such as
the and auxiliaries such as be, have, do).
 
 
By 6 months, infants recognize familiar words,
including their own names.
By 9 months, they recognize sound patterns
for words of their language. The ability to
perceive words as units lays the foundation for
the ability to pro­duce single-word utterances.
 
How infants produce speech
 
Calling attention: Crying
The universal strategy of all babies everywhere is
crying.
Crying comes in regular bursts - there are pauses
in between, and each sound burst falls in pitch as
it goes on. And if the crying isn't immediately
responded to, babies turn up the volume. These
three ingredients - a rhythm of recurring sounds,
modulation of pitch, and modulation of volume -
lay the groundwork for speech.
 
Communicating before words: Cooing
 
Babies are getting used to the shape of their vocal tract,
figuring out where their tongue fits in your mouth, and
learning to coordinate breathing and making sound.
By 2 months, they've progressed to cooing. They
experiment making consonant-like and vowel-like sounds –
these are not actual consonants and vowels - and they're
soon stringing them together in longer sequences.
Somewhere between the ages of 5 and 8 months, their
natural ability as a phonetician bursts forth. Babies can
produce all sorts of speech sounds, including ones that are
not in their target language.
 
 
Research conducted by Catherine Snow
indicates that adults provide turn-taking
instructions to young infants. She reports the
following interaction between a mother and
her infant daughter of 3 months. Notice that
the mother's feedback gives her daughter
implicit instruction on turn-taking.
 
 
Daughter: (smiles)
Mother: 
Oh, what a nice little smile. Yes, isn't
that nice? There. There's a nice tittle smile.
Daughter: (burps)
Mother: 
What a nice little wind as well. Yes,
that's better, isn't it? Yes.
Daughter: (vocalizes)
Mother: 
There's a nice noise.
 
 
By the age of 6 months, infants have figured
out how to get adult attention through a
mixture of vocalizations and gestures. But the
adults are still doing all the work to create the
conversation, interpreting any sounds the
infant makes - from cooing to burping - as a
conversational turn.
 
Babbling
 
Next infants start to aim on their target language. By
the age of 6 to 7 months, they're babbling away, and
the sounds that they are using are getting closer and
closer to being the sounds of their language.
They repeat similar syllables and sounds over and over
again: 
ba, ba, bi, bi, bu, bu
.
The ability to detect and produce sound distinctions
not found in the infants’ language starts to fade away:
By 9 months, their speech 
perception
 is focused on the
sounds of their target language, and by 10 months
their speech 
pro­duction
 locks onto their target
language as well.
 
From phonemes to syllables to word
 
1. Stop consonants (
/p, t, k, b, d, g, m, n, n/) and
glides (y, w/) 
 around 7 months
2. Vowels 
 around 24 months
3. 
Fricatives /s, z/, affricates /ts, dz/, and liquids
/r, l/ 
 around 30-36 months
Until they attain the motor control that allows
them to produce all the sounds of their
language, kids often omit or substitute sounds
 
Substitution
 
Kids often voice consonants in initial position,
and devoice consonants m the final position. So
p
ie 
comes out as [
b
ay] and 
kno
b
 comes out as
[na
p
].
And until they figure out how to produce
frication, they often substitute a stop for a
fricative. So 
knife
 comes out as [naib], and 
bus
comes out as [b
d].
And because /r/and/l/ come in so late, kids often
substitute those sounds with (y] and (w]. So
rabbit 
comes out as [w
bit].
 
Omission
 
Kids often omit the final consonant of a word. So 
ball
comes out as [ba], 
boot
 as [bu].
For words with more than one syllable, kids acquiring
English, which has a stress-based intonation, often
omit the weak (unstressed) syllable. So 
bye
-bye 
comes
out as (bab]. 
hel
lo
 
comes out as [hwow]. 
Ste
vie 
comes
out as [iv], and 
a
way
 
comes out as [wei].
This also means that they often omit grammatical
words such as 
the 
and 
a 
(which are almost always
unstressed). So a sentence like 
He catches the pig 
may
come out as 'He catches pig.'
 
 
ln addition to learning the phonemes of their target
language, kids also have to figure out how the sounds
combine with each other.
Kids' first words are often single CV syllables, like [ga], [da],
[ba], [ma], and [na].
Then they move onto sequences of CV syllables, then CVC
syllables, then syllables with long vowels (CVV, CVVC).
And when they start making CVC syllables, they often
produce forms where the two consonants have the same
place of articulation, for example (tin] (where both [t] and
[n] are alveolar) or (pom] (where both [p] and [m] are
labial).
 
Language after the first year of life
 
After infants get their first words out (at around
12 months), they start combining words with
each other (at around 18 months) and then move
onto more complex sentences (at around 24
months).
By 3 years old, infants have a pretty good
understanding of how their language forms
words (morphology) and sentences (syntax) and
how to use language appropriately (pragmatics).
 
 
Linguists give names to some of the stages
that kids go through:
The 
one-word stage 
(12 to 18 months) is also
called the 
holophrastic 
or 
whole sentence stage
.
It's followed by the 
two-word stage 
(18 to 24
months)
and then the 
telegraphic speech stage 
(24 to 30
months).
 
Major developmental stage:
one-word stage (holophrastic)
 
Naming
Children can be said to have learned their first word when
(1) they are able to utter a recognizable speech form, and
when (2) this is done in conjunction with some object or
event in the environment.
 
First words have been reported as appearing in children
from as young as 4 months to as old as 18 months, or
even older. On average, it would seem that children utter
their first word around the age of 10 or 12 months.
 
 
Holophrastic function
Children do not only use single words to refer to objects;
they also use single words to express complex thoughts
that involve those objects.
The child uses a single word to express the thought for
which mature speakers will use a whole sentence. 
 holo
(whole) phrastic (phrase, sentence)
Example: 
A young child who has lost its mother in a
department store may cry out ‘mama’, meaning ‘I want
mama’. Or a child may point to a shoe and say ‘mama’,
meaning ‘The shoe belongs to mama’.
 
Two-word stage
 
Around 2 years of age (18 to 24 months) or so children
begin to produce two- and three-word utterances.
They know about 50 words, and they use two-word
combinations - this is the beginning of syntax.
For example, 
Doggie bark 
for 'The dog is barking'
expresses an agent-action semantic relationship and a
subject-verb syntactic relationship.
And 
Daddy hat
, for 'Daddy's hat', expresses a
possessor-possessed semantic relationship and a
genitive-noun syntactic relationship.
By this time, they can walk, feed themselves, and
scribble lines with crayons.
 
Knitting together sound, meaning, and
structure
 
There's a rapid vocabulary expansion: At 24
months children know about 400 words, by 30
months they have around 900 words, by 36
months they're at 1,200 words, and by 48 months
they're hovering at 1,500 words.
There's also a rapid growth of syntax – children
now begin to master the forms of words, the
rules of word order, the placement of negation,
and the forms of passives, questions, and relative
clauses.
 
 
Learning the forms of words
In early stages of language acquisition, kids leave out
grammatical words and word endings, in particular,
inflectional suffixes.
Figuring word order
Between 24 and 30 months, kids figure out the rules for
basic sentences. They use the subject-predicate pattern,
for example, 
I good boy 
for 'I'm a good boy'. They also
use the subject-verb-object pattern (
Daddy like book 
for
'Daddy likes the book'), which they embellish on with an
adverb (
Man ride bus today 
for 'The man rode the bus
today').
 
 
Going from active to passive
By the age of two, kids start to use the passive-live
sentence, but they don't fully master the passive
construction until they're three. Eve Clark reports the
following sentences:
Adult models:
 
My temperature was taken by the doctor.
 
I want to see my bottle getting fixed.
Child's version (26 months):
 
I took my temperature from the doctor.
 
I want see my bottle getting fix.
 
 
Asking questions
As documented by Edward Klima and Ursula Bellugi,
children progress in their mastery of both yes/no
questions and content-questions in the following
way:
Stage I : 
see hole? 
and 
where kitty?
Stage 2: 
you want eat? 
and 
where my mitten?
Stage 3: 
will you help me? 
and 
where the other Joe
will drive?
 
 
Forming relative clause
By the age of two, kids unpack relative clauses
into their component clauses.
Dan Slobin and Charles Welsh report the
following relative clauses produced by a 26-
month-old child. It will take another two years
for kids to master the form of relative clauses,
but by the age of four they match the adult
model.
 
 
Adult model:
 
The owl who eats candy runs fast.
 
The man who I saw yesterday got wet.
Child's version (26 months):
 
Owl eat candy and he run fast.
 
I saw the man and he got wet.
 
 
Achieving a basic grammar
By the age of five, kids have mastered the basics
of their grammar.
They can use language to talk about language,
define words, and correct themselves.
The rest is fine-tuning - vocabulary continues to
increase (at a slower rate), and kids become
more sensitive to stylistic variation.
Slide Note
Embed
Share

At birth, infants cannot comprehend or produce speech, but by the age of 4, they have acquired vocabulary and grammatical rules to form various sentence structures. From vocalization to babbling and then to speech, infants progress through distinct stages in language development. Understanding how infants perceive speech involves overcoming challenges related to segmentation, invariance, and language selection. The journey from babbling to uttering first words is a significant milestone in language acquisition.

  • Language acquisition
  • Children
  • Psycholinguistics
  • Speech perception

Uploaded on Aug 14, 2024 | 0 Views


Download Presentation

Please find below an Image/Link to download the presentation.

The content on the website is provided AS IS for your information and personal use only. It may not be sold, licensed, or shared on other websites without obtaining consent from the author.If you encounter any issues during the download, it is possible that the publisher has removed the file from their server.

You are allowed to download the files provided on this website for personal or commercial use, subject to the condition that they are used lawfully. All files are the property of their respective owners.

The content on the website is provided AS IS for your information and personal use only. It may not be sold, licensed, or shared on other websites without obtaining consent from the author.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. How children learn language Psycholinguistics Meeting 8

  2. At birth we cannot comprehend speech, nor can we produce speech. Yet, by the age of 4 years we have learned vocabulary and grammatical rules for creating a variety of sentence structures including negatives, questions, and relative clauses. Although 4-year-olds still have passives and some other elaborate syntactic structures to learn, along with a never-ending stock of vocabulary items, they have already overcome the most difficult obstacles in language learning.

  3. Vocalization to babbling Prior to uttering speech sounds, infants make a variety of sounds crying, cooing, gurgling. Infants everywhere seem to make the same variety of sounds, even children who are born deaf. Around the seventh month, children ordinarily begin to babble, to produce what may be described as repeated syllables ( syllabic reduplication ), e.g. baba , momo , panpan .

  4. While most of the syllables are of the basic Consonant + Vowel type ( baba and momo ), some consist of closed syllables of the simple Consonant + Vowel + Consonant variety ( panpan ).

  5. Babbling to speech It is from the advanced stage of babbling that children move into uttering their first words. Often this occurs at around 1 year of age but can occur much earlier or much later.

  6. How infants perceive speech About 3 months before birth, you begin sensing sound and draw on this experience from the outset. You begin life with a preference for listening to speech over other complex sounds. How do infants break into the speech stream? They face three challenges:

  7. The segmentation problem: How do infants figure out that a stream of speech can be segmented into phonemes? The invariance problem: How do infants identify a stable set of phonemes from a signal that is full of variation? For example, /p/ is pronounced differently according to whether it appears at the beginning or end of a word: compare pit (where /p/ is aspirated) to tip (where /p/ isn't aspirated). Add to that the fact that each speaker has a slightly different pronunciation, and that even the same speaker doesn't pronounce the same word twice in the same way. The language problem: How do infants figure out which set of sounds belong to their target language?

  8. Initially young infants can discriminate (perceive whether two sounds are the same or different) phonetic sounds from any human language, even ones they haven't been exposed to. They retain this ability until the age of about 8 months, at which point they show a preference for the sounds of their target language. Infants pay more attention to the sounds that are most frequent. Being able to discriminate the sounds of a target language and to identify the most frequent ones are the first steps in solving the segmentation and to language problems.

  9. By 8 months, infants are expert statisticians and keep score of which sounds hang out together. They detect regularities about which sounds are likely to occur with which other sounds, the typical stress patterns on words, and the difference between content words (such as verbs and nouns) and function words (articles such as the and auxiliaries such as be, have, do).

  10. By 6 months, infants recognize familiar words, including their own names. By 9 months, they recognize sound patterns for words of their language. The ability to perceive words as units lays the foundation for the ability to produce single-word utterances.

  11. How infants produce speech Calling attention: Crying The universal strategy of all babies everywhere is crying. Crying comes in regular bursts - there are pauses in between, and each sound burst falls in pitch as it goes on. And if the crying isn't immediately responded to, babies turn up the volume. These three ingredients - a rhythm of recurring sounds, modulation of pitch, and modulation of volume - lay the groundwork for speech.

  12. Communicating before words: Cooing Babies are getting used to the shape of their vocal tract, figuring out where their tongue fits in your mouth, and learning to coordinate breathing and making sound. By 2 months, they've progressed to cooing. They experiment making consonant-like and vowel-like sounds these are not actual consonants and vowels - and they're soon stringing them together in longer sequences. Somewhere between the ages of 5 and 8 months, their natural ability as a phonetician bursts forth. Babies can produce all sorts of speech sounds, including ones that are not in their target language.

  13. Research conducted by Catherine Snow indicates that adults provide turn-taking instructions to young infants. She reports the following interaction between a mother and her infant daughter of 3 months. Notice that the mother's feedback gives her daughter implicit instruction on turn-taking.

  14. Daughter: (smiles) Mother: Oh, what a nice little smile. Yes, isn't that nice? There. There's a nice tittle smile. Daughter: (burps) Mother: What a nice little wind as well. Yes, that's better, isn't it? Yes. Daughter: (vocalizes) Mother: There's a nice noise.

  15. By the age of 6 months, infants have figured out how to get adult attention through a mixture of vocalizations and gestures. But the adults are still doing all the work to create the conversation, interpreting any sounds the infant makes - from cooing to burping - as a conversational turn.

  16. Babbling Next infants start to aim on their target language. By the age of 6 to 7 months, they're babbling away, and the sounds that they are using are getting closer and closer to being the sounds of their language. They repeat similar syllables and sounds over and over again: ba, ba, bi, bi, bu, bu. The ability to detect and produce sound distinctions not found in the infants language starts to fade away: By 9 months, their speech perception is focused on the sounds of their target language, and by 10 months their speech production locks onto their target language as well.

  17. From phonemes to syllables to word 1. Stop consonants (/p, t, k, b, d, g, m, n, n/) and glides (y, w/) around 7 months 2. Vowels around 24 months 3. Fricatives /s, z/, affricates /ts, dz/, and liquids /r, l/ around 30-36 months Until they attain the motor control that allows them to produce all the sounds of their language, kids often omit or substitute sounds

  18. Substitution Kids often voice consonants in initial position, and devoice consonants m the final position. So pie comes out as [bay] and knob comes out as [nap]. And until they figure out how to produce frication, they often substitute a stop for a fricative. So knife comes out as [naib], and bus comes out as [b d]. And because /r/and/l/ come in so late, kids often substitute those sounds with (y] and (w]. So rabbit comes out as [w bit].

  19. Omission Kids often omit the final consonant of a word. So ball comes out as [ba], boot as [bu]. For words with more than one syllable, kids acquiring English, which has a stress-based intonation, often omit the weak (unstressed) syllable. So bye-bye comes out as (bab]. hello comes out as [hwow]. Stevie comes out as [iv], and away comes out as [wei]. This also means that they often omit grammatical words such as the and a (which are almost always unstressed). So a sentence like He catches the pig may come out as 'He catches pig.'

  20. ln addition to learning the phonemes of their target language, kids also have to figure out how the sounds combine with each other. Kids' first words are often single CV syllables, like [ga], [da], [ba], [ma], and [na]. Then they move onto sequences of CV syllables, then CVC syllables, then syllables with long vowels (CVV, CVVC). And when they start making CVC syllables, they often produce forms where the two consonants have the same place of articulation, for example (tin] (where both [t] and [n] are alveolar) or (pom] (where both [p] and [m] are labial).

  21. Language after the first year of life After infants get their first words out (at around 12 months), they start combining words with each other (at around 18 months) and then move onto more complex sentences (at around 24 months). By 3 years old, infants have a pretty good understanding of how their language forms words (morphology) and sentences (syntax) and how to use language appropriately (pragmatics).

  22. Linguists give names to some of the stages that kids go through: The one-word stage (12 to 18 months) is also called the holophrastic or whole sentence stage. It's followed by the two-word stage (18 to 24 months) and then the telegraphic speech stage (24 to 30 months).

  23. Major developmental stage: one-word stage (holophrastic) Naming Children can be said to have learned their first word when (1) they are able to utter a recognizable speech form, and when (2) this is done in conjunction with some object or event in the environment. First words have been reported as appearing in children from as young as 4 months to as old as 18 months, or even older. On average, it would seem that children utter their first word around the age of 10 or 12 months.

  24. Holophrastic function Children do not only use single words to refer to objects; they also use single words to express complex thoughts that involve those objects. The child uses a single word to express the thought for which mature speakers will use a whole sentence. holo (whole) phrastic (phrase, sentence) Example: A young child who has lost its mother in a department store may cry out mama , meaning I want mama . Or a child may point to a shoe and say mama , meaning The shoe belongs to mama .

  25. Two-word stage Around 2 years of age (18 to 24 months) or so children begin to produce two- and three-word utterances. They know about 50 words, and they use two-word combinations - this is the beginning of syntax. For example, Doggie bark for 'The dog is barking' expresses an agent-action semantic relationship and a subject-verb syntactic relationship. And Daddy hat, for 'Daddy's hat', expresses a possessor-possessed semantic relationship and a genitive-noun syntactic relationship. By this time, they can walk, feed themselves, and scribble lines with crayons.

  26. Knitting together sound, meaning, and structure There's a rapid vocabulary expansion: At 24 months children know about 400 words, by 30 months they have around 900 words, by 36 months they're at 1,200 words, and by 48 months they're hovering at 1,500 words. There's also a rapid growth of syntax children now begin to master the forms of words, the rules of word order, the placement of negation, and the forms of passives, questions, and relative clauses.

  27. Learning the forms of words In early stages of language acquisition, kids leave out grammatical words and word endings, in particular, inflectional suffixes. Figuring word order Between 24 and 30 months, kids figure out the rules for basic sentences. They use the subject-predicate pattern, for example, I good boy for 'I'm a good boy'. They also use the subject-verb-object pattern (Daddy like book for 'Daddy likes the book'), which they embellish on with an adverb (Man ride bus today for 'The man rode the bus today').

  28. Going from active to passive By the age of two, kids start to use the passive-live sentence, but they don't fully master the passive construction until they're three. Eve Clark reports the following sentences: Adult models: My temperature was taken by the doctor. I want to see my bottle getting fixed. Child's version (26 months): I took my temperature from the doctor. I want see my bottle getting fix.

  29. Asking questions As documented by Edward Klima and Ursula Bellugi, children progress in their mastery of both yes/no questions and content-questions in the following way: Stage I : see hole? and where kitty? Stage 2: you want eat? and where my mitten? Stage 3: will you help me? and where the other Joe will drive?

  30. Forming relative clause By the age of two, kids unpack relative clauses into their component clauses. Dan Slobin and Charles Welsh report the following relative clauses produced by a 26- month-old child. It will take another two years for kids to master the form of relative clauses, but by the age of four they match the adult model.

  31. Adult model: The owl who eats candy runs fast. The man who I saw yesterday got wet. Child's version (26 months): Owl eat candy and he run fast. I saw the man and he got wet.

  32. Achieving a basic grammar By the age of five, kids have mastered the basics of their grammar. They can use language to talk about language, define words, and correct themselves. The rest is fine-tuning - vocabulary continues to increase (at a slower rate), and kids become more sensitive to stylistic variation.

More Related Content

giItT1WQy@!-/#giItT1WQy@!-/#giItT1WQy@!-/#giItT1WQy@!-/#giItT1WQy@!-/#giItT1WQy@!-/#